Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 1996 - Abstracts

Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 1996
TitleSubjectAuthors
Achieving parity: the role of automatic reinforcement. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workPalmer, David C.
A computer touch-screen apparatus for training visual discrimination in rats.Sociology and social workDougher, Michael J., Markham, Michael R., Butt, Allen E.
Assessment and choice: an operant simulation of foraging in patches.Sociology and social workGlanz, William E., Roche, John P., Stubbs, D. Alan
Behavioral variability in SHR and WKY rats as a function of rearing environment and reinforcement of contingency. (spontaneously hypertensive rat; Wistar-Kyoto control rat)Sociology and social workNeuringer, Allen, Hunziker, M.H.L., Saldana, R. Lisa
Blocking, unblocking, and overexpectation in autoshaping with pigeons.Sociology and social workKhallad, Yacoub, Moore, Jay
Can the naming hypothesis be falsified? (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workPilgrim, Carol
Changeover behavior and preference in concurrent schedules.Sociology and social workWilliams, Ben A., Bell, Matthew C.
Choice as a function of reinforcement ratios in delayed matching to sample.Sociology and social workFantino, Edmund, Hartl, Josef A.
Choice with certain and uncertain reinforcers in an adjusting-delay procedure.Sociology and social workMazur, James E.
Closed-economy multiple-schedule performance: effects of deprivation and session duration.Sociology and social workDavison, Michael, Elliffe, Douglas
Cocaine and food deprivation: effects on food-reinforced fixed-ratio performance in pigeons.Sociology and social workBranch, Marc N., Hughes, Christine E., Pitts, Raymond C.
Commitment using punishment.(two-link choice procedure for pigeons)Sociology and social workGreen, Leonard, Rachlin, Howard
Concurrent choice: effects of overall reinforcer rate and the temporal distribution of reinforcers.Sociology and social workElliffe, Douglas, Alsop, Brent
Concurrent-schedule performance in dairy cows: persistent undermatching.Sociology and social workFoster, T. Mary, Temple, William (English archbishop), Poling, Alan, Robertson, Brett, Nair, Vijayen
Concurrent schedules: quantifying the aversiveness of noise.Sociology and social workFoster, T. Mary, Temple, William (English archbishop), McAdie, Tina M.
Detecting a nonevent: delayed presence-versus-absence discrimination in pigeons.Sociology and social workWixted, John T., Dougherty, Deirdra H.
Developing a theory of derived stimulus relations. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workHayes, Steven C.
Differences in delay, not ratios, control choice in concurrent chains.Sociology and social workFantino, Edmund, Savastano, Hernan I.
Differences in the effect of Pavlovian contingencies upon behavioral momentum using auditory versus visual stimuli.Sociology and social workMauro, Benjamin C., Mace, F. Charles
Discrimination of methadone and cocaine by pigeons without explicit discrimination training.Sociology and social workMiller, Mark A., Schaal, David W., Reilly, Mark P., McDonald, Michael P.
Drug discrimination under a concurrent schedule.Sociology and social workMcMillan, D. E., Snodgrass, Samuel H.
Dynamics of time discrimination: II. The effects of multiple impulses.Sociology and social workHiga, Jennifer J.
Dynamics of waiting in pigeons.Sociology and social workStaddon, J.E.R., Wynne, C.D.L., Delius, J.D.
Effects of cocaine on briefly signaled versus completely signaled delays to reinforcement.Sociology and social workBranch, Marc N., Walker, Diana J.
Effects of D-amphetamine on response acquisition with immediate and delayed reinforcement.Sociology and social workPoling, Alan, LeSage, Mark G., Byrne, Tom
Effects of food-pellet size on rate, latency, and topography of autoshaped key pecks and gapes in pigeons.Sociology and social workPloog, Bertram O., Zeigler, H.P.
Effects of reinforcement amount on attack induced under a fixed-interval schedule in pigeons.Sociology and social workPitts, Raymond C., Malagodi, E.F.
Effects of response type on pigeons' sensitivity to variation in reinforcer amount and reinforcer delay.Sociology and social workLogue, A.W., Chelonis, John J.
Effects of spatial rearrangement of object components on picture recognition in pigeons.Sociology and social workWasserman, Edward A., Kirkpatrick-Steger, Kim, Biederman, Irving
Equivalence relations, naming, and generalized symmetry. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workCarr, D., Blackman, D.E.
Experimental analysis of naming behavior cannot explain naming capacity. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workHarnad, Stevan
Food and water intake as functions of resource consumption costs in a closed economy.Sociology and social workJohnson, Deanne F., Collier, George, Mathis, Carl E.
Fred S. Keller: rememberings.Sociology and social workSidman, Murray
Goodbye teacher, good old friend.(behavior analyst Fred S. Keller)Sociology and social workTodorov, Joao Claudio
Humans' choices in situations of time-based diminishing returns: effects of fixed-interval duration and progressive-interval step size.Sociology and social workJacobs, Eric A., Hackenberg, Timothy D.
Human signal-detection performance: effects of signal presentation probabilities and reinforcer distributions.Sociology and social workJohnstone, Victoria, Alsop, Brent
Investigating behavioral dynamics with a fixed-time extinction schedule and linear analysis.Sociology and social workKessel, Robert, Palya, Wliiiam L., Walter, Don, Lucke, Robert
JEAB and JABA on the world wide web: a report to readers.(Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis)Sociology and social workPreston, Ray A., Laties, Victor G., Inglis, Geoffrey B., Pittelli, Randal L.
Joint control and word-object bidirectionality. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workLowenkron, Barry
Learning names may help to make the right connections. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workDickins, David W., Bentall, Richard P.
Listener behavior and ostensive learning. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workStemmer, Nathan
Listening with understanding and speaking with meaning. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workHayes, Linda J.
Methodological issues in the study of naming. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workGalizio, Mark
Names as constituents of sentences: an omission. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workPlace, Ullin T.
Naming and equivalence relations. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workSaunders, Kathryn J., Spradlin, Joseph E.
Naming as a facilitator of discrimination. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workDube, William V., McIlvane, William J.
Naming as a technical term: sacrificing behavior analysis at the altar of popularity. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workBarnes, Dermot
Naming is not (necessary for) stimulus equivalence. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workSaunders, Richard R., Green, Gina
Naming, meaning, and verbal operants. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workRose, Julio C. de
Naming, stimulus equivalence, and conditioned hearing. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workDugdale, Neil
Natural contingencies in the creation of naming as a higher order behavior class. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workCatania, A. Charles
On naming and symbolic behavior: in the beginning was the name? (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workHackenberg, Timothy D., Vaidya, Manish
On the experimental analysis of naming and the formation of stimulus classes. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workStromer, Robert
On the origins of misguided theories of naming and other symbolic behavior. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workWhitehurst, Grover J.
On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior.Sociology and social workLowe, C. Fergus, Horne, Pauline J.
Pointing at smaller food amounts in an analogue of Boysen and Berntson's (1995) procedure.Sociology and social workFujita, Kazuo, Silberberg, Alan
Postscript to an anthology of reviews.Sociology and social workHineline, Philip N., Catania, A. Charles
Preference between variable-ratio and fixed-ratio schedules: local and extended relations.Sociology and social workHineline, Philip N., Field, Douglas P., Tonneau, Francois, Ahearn, William
Procrastination by pigeons: preference for larger, more delayed work requirements.Sociology and social workMazur, James E.
Reflections on naming and other symbolic behavior. (response to articles by Bob Remington et al. in this issue, p. 243)Sociology and social workLowe, C. Fergus, Horne, Pauline J.
Relative sensitivity to reinforcer amount and delay in a self-control choice situation.Sociology and social workIto, Masato, Oyama, Mariko
Representing within-session response rates proportionally and entirely.Sociology and social workSchaal, David W.
Residence time and choice in concurrent foraging schedules.Sociology and social workDavison, Michael, Jones, B. Maxwell
Resistance to change as a function of stimulus-reinforcer and location-reinforcer contingencies.Sociology and social workNevin, John A., McLean, Anthony P., Campbell-Tie, Philippa
Response-dependent prechoice effects on foraging-related choice.Sociology and social workFantino, Edmund, Williams, Wendy A.
Response-independent food delivery and behavioral resistance to change.Sociology and social workHarper, David N.
Resurgence of derived stimulus relations.Sociology and social workHayes, Steven C., Wilson, Kelly G.
Separate repertoires or naming? (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workMichael, Jack
Separating the effects of trial-specific and average sample-stimulus duration in delayed matching to sample in pigeons.Sociology and social workWixted, John T., Hartl, Josef A., Dougherty, Deirdra A.
Speed analysis of stimulus equivalence.Sociology and social workChase, Philip N., Spencer, Thomas J.
Stimulus effects on behavior allocation in three-alternative choice.Sociology and social workDavison, Michael
Substitution and caloric regulation in a closed economy.Sociology and social workHursh, Steven R., Bauman, Richard A., Raslear, Thomas G., Shurtleff, David, Simmons, Laurence
Temporal control by progressive-interval schedules of reinforcement.Sociology and social workBaron, Alan, Leinenweber, Antoinette, Nietzel, Shannon M.
The evidence for naming as a cause or facilitator of equivalence class formation. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workFields, Lanny
The evolution of naming - just so! (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workRemington, Bob
The origins of naming: a critique of self-listening. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workChase, Philip N.
Thought without naming. (response to article by Pauline J. Horne and C. Fergus Lowe in this issue, p. 185)Sociology and social workPelaez-Nogueras, Martha
Token reinforcement, choice, and self-control in pigeons.Sociology and social workJackson, Kevin, Hackenberg, Timothy D.
Types of responding in a signal-detection task.Sociology and social workAlsop, Brent, Rowley, Rachael
Verbal self-reports of emergent relations in a stimulus equivalence procedure.Sociology and social workCritchfield, Thomas S., Lane, Scott D.
Within-session changes in responding during autoshaping and automaintenance procedures.Sociology and social workMcSweeney, Frances K., Weatherly, Jeffrey N., Swindell, Samantha
Within-session changes in responding during concurrent schedules with different reinforcers in the components.Sociology and social workMcSweeney, Frances K., Weatherly, Jeffrey N., Swindell, Samantha
Within-session changes in responding during concurrent variable-interval schedules.Sociology and social workMcSweeney, Frances K., Weatherly, Jeffrey N., Swindell, Samantha
Within-session response patterns on conjoint variable-interval variable-time schedules.Sociology and social workMcSweeney, Frances K., Weatherly, Jeffrey N., Swindell, Samantha
This website is not affiliated with document authors or copyright owners. This page is provided for informational purposes only. Unintentional errors are possible.